Recently I wrote a tongue-in-cheek piece in which I statistically analyzed 5,500 emails exchanged with my boyfriend over the past four years. Thanks so much to the many friends who said nice things and shared the piece; it meant a lot to me, because the internet evidently has decided I’m undateable. Here’s a sample of responses:
“Don't be surprised if the BF dumps her.”
“That poor bastard.”
“Maybe he doesn't miss her because she's a lunatic.”
“I was originally going to suggest the author break up now and start collecting cats.”
“I'm guessing that this couple is very young and have a trivial understanding of love.”
“I'm thinking the guy should run, and fast. This is author is obsessive...”
I am, of course, obsessive -- note the title of this blog -- but about statistics, not my boyfriend. And so when I read these responses (after I walked into my boyfriend’s bedroom and asked worriedly, “Have I been totally horrible to you for four years? I haven’t, have I? Do you want me to make you a waffle?” [1]) I began to wonder why people assumed my obsession was with my relationship rather than with math. Would people have responded differently to the essay, I wondered, had I been male?
To answer this question, I created two abridged versions of the essay which were identical except that one was supposedly written by a man with a girlfriend and one was supposedly written by a woman with a boyfriend. (One interesting follow-up would be to run a version of this experiment with same-sex couples.) I put the essays on Mechanical Turk, a site social scientists use to run surveys, and asked people to rate the author along various dimensions:
with a total of 23 adjectives. (You can read more about the survey methodology in footnote [2]). Below, I refer to the person who read and rated the essay as the “reader” and the person who wrote the essay as the “author”.
It turns out that gender does affect how people respond; both the gender of the author and the gender of the reader were significantly correlated with the adjectives that were used [3]. Female authors were described differently than male authors. Below I plot each word by how much people thought it described the male author (horizontal axis) and female author (vertical axis). The left plot shows the results for female readers, and the right plot shows the results for male readers. (Apologies for the small font; zoom in.)
If a word lies near the diagonal black line, it means that people used it to describe male and female authors equally often; I’ve highlighted in red words which are especially far from the black line (p < .05, t-test; I didn’t use multiple-hypothesis correction because I just wanted to visually highlight the most skewed words [4]). Male readers rated female authors as more “dangerous”, “bitchy”, “aggressive”, “crazy”, and “possessive” and less “smart” than male authors; female readers described male authors as “genius” and “weird” more often than female authors. When we combine results for both male and female readers, female authors were more often described as “bitchy”, “crazy”, “dangerous”, and “aggressive”, and male authors were more often described as “genius”.
Here’s a slightly different way of looking at the data. Below I plot each adjective by how often male readers used it to describe the female author (horizontal axis) and how often female readers used it to describe the female author (vertical axis). Male and female readers differed significantly in how they reacted to the essay written by a female author.
Female readers were more likely to describe the female author as “likeable” and “dateable”, whereas male readers were more likely to describe her as “obsessive”, “possessive”, “aggressive”, “autistic”, “robotic”, and “dangerous” (thanks, y’all). (Interestingly, there were no large differences in how male and female readers reacted to male authors.)
These results are intriguing and not what I expected, so feel free to comment or email me if you have explanations or follow-up projects.
One final thought. This experiment identifies a fairly clean case, I think, in which I was treated differently because of my gender. In general, it is very hard to be sure whether treatment is due to gender. For example, I have been condescended to hundreds of times, but I have no idea which (if any) of those occurred because I was female. Ellen Pao’s defeat in her gender discrimination lawsuit -- and, more broadly, the low success rate of plaintiffs in such cases -- also illustrates the difficulty of proving that treatment is due to gender. The difficulty of proof implies, I think, two things: a) we shouldn’t dismiss an allegation of gender bias as false merely because it is unprovable; b) we should keep thinking of creative ways to prove bias (Experiments? Class actions?)
Notes:
[1] Perhaps this is too obvious to mention, but when you write an internet comment about the author of a personal essay, remember that the author may actually read it. (I have spoken to other authors who do this as well.) I want to continue to write personal essays about statistics because a) most people find pure math dry and b) one of the best writers I ever got to work with yelled at me repeatedly to be more intimate (“face your dragons! face your dragons!”) But like, it’s a bit of a downer to be told by random strangers on the internet that you ought to be dumped -- and obviously, I can deal with it and it’s your right to make such comments, but I would still think about what you’re really accomplishing.
[2] I gave the survey to 200 people on Mechanical Turk and randomized whether each person read the male or female version of the essay. I asked two comprehension questions to make sure people actually read the essay and filtered out people who gave incorrect responses. After filtering, I was left with 94 responses from male readers and 90 from female readers.
[3] I ran the regression author_gender ~ adj_1_score + adj_2_score + … + adj_23_score; then I did a joint F-test on all the coefficients, which yielded a p-value of 7.2 * 10-4. Similarly, the joint F-test on reader_gender ~ adj_1_score + adj_2_score + … + adj_23_score yielded a p-value of 7.5 * 10-4. These regressions are a little unintuitive because their structure implies that adjectives affect gender rather than the other way around, but they show that the adjective scores are significantly correlated with gender of both author and reader.
[4] So you should take any individual word with a grain of salt -- but the difference between words as a whole is highly significant, and the patterns are pretty consistent.
I liked the original article. I loved this :).
ReplyDeleteI do have a few thoughts, if you'd care to dig a bit deeper.
1) How correlated are the negative associations? The point here is to answer the following: Is it that a few men (or women) hold strongly negative views which drives the difference, or perhaps its that the general population that holds slightly more negative views?
2) It feels like there is a missing control. What would men and women rate about a gender neutral author? I realize this is a hard experiment to do because of the nature of the underlying piece. I think this is important because it allows you to segregate a positive male bias against a negative female bias, or at least give you a data point to look at.
Thanks! Interesting questions.
Delete1. Not exactly sure what you mean by "how correlated are the negative associations"? What are we looking at correlations between?
2. Gender neutral author is an interesting idea. Not sure how you would achieve this -- if you say "boyfriend" or "girlfriend" people will make assumptions. Perhaps you could say "partner" but then people may assume you're not straight, which could affect responses. But it would be interesting to ask people what gender they think the neutral author is.
I think what he make be asking with "How correlated are the negative associations?" is something akin to: "Did the median male rate you as more possessive, or is it just 1/4 of the guys rated you as extremely possessive?" Obviously, extrapolate that to other adjectives, genders, and proportions. Though if it is just a subset, it might be worth knowing whether it is the same subset of readers using extreme values on all (or related subgroups) of adjectives.
DeleteFor what it's worth, I thought your original article was funny and charmingly self-aware.
ReplyDeleteThanks :) Good to see someone didn't take it too seriously. (Speaking of which, nice name.)
DeleteI just discovered your writing and I think it's brilliant! Personally, the mental image I had of you post the article was only that I thought you were "intellectually curious". And brave! To be able to look the results in the eye come what may, I find that very brave and admirable.
ReplyDeleteThanks! Always nice to hear :)
DeleteThank you for sharing such a brilliant article.
ReplyDeleteดรีมแคลชเชอร์
ดรีมแคลชเชอร์
ดรีมแคลชเชอร์
that’s awesome.
ReplyDeleteดรีมแคลชเชอร์
ดรีมแคลชเชอร์
ดรีมแคลชเชอร์
it’s so amazing.
ReplyDeleteดรีมแคลชเชอร์
ดรีมแคลชเชอร์
ดรีมแคลชเชอร์
ดรีมแคลชเชอร์
found so many interesting stuff in your blog especially its discussion. From the tons of comments on your articles, I guess I am not the only one having all the enjoyment here! keep up the good work... สล็อตออนไลน์
ReplyDeleteWhat a sensational blog! This blog is too much amazing in all aspects. Especially, it looks awesome and the content available on it is utmost qualitative.สล็อตทรูวอเลท
ReplyDelete서울출장샵
ReplyDelete광주출장샵
광주출장샵
인천출장샵
인천출장샵
횡성출장샵
세종출장샵
부천콜걸
ReplyDelete화성콜걸
안산콜걸
안양콜걸
평택콜걸
시흥콜걸
김포콜걸
경기광주콜걸
Are you looking to elevate your travel with Asiana Airlines? Need help on How to cancel and refund at Asiana Airlines Flight? For assistance, contact Asiana Airlines at OTA+1 800 970 3794 (no wait).
ReplyDeleteHow to cancel and refund at Asiana Airlines Flight?